Joined: February 11th, 2012, 7:29 am

January 23rd, 2018, 4:20 am #11

Studies for & against Social Behaviour in Smilodon

Journal Reference:
McCall S1, Naples V, Martin L. Assessing behavior in extinct animals: was Smilodon social? Brain Behav Evol. 2003;61(3):159-64.

Abstract
It has been suggested that saber-tooth species such as Smilodon fatalis were social because partially healed skeletal injuries were found at Rancho La Brea, California. This conclusion assumes injured animals would die without help. This paper will rebut assertions of sociality. First, cats use metabolic reserves to heal quickly without feeding. Second, dehydration is a more profound limitation than starvation as prey carcasses only provide a quarter of necessary water. Injured animals must be mobile enough to find water or die of dehydration. Their presence in a tar pit also strongly suggests locomotion. Finally, the relatively small brain found in Smilodon is not consistent with sociality. Another argument for sociality has been the large ratio of Smilodon to other species in the La Brea tar pits. However, the remains of a non-social species, the Golden eagle (Aquila chrysaetus), are about as common as Smilodon. Contrariwise, the highly social grey wolf (Canis lupus) and coyote (Canis latrans) are extremely rare. Available evidence does not support sociality in Smilodon.

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12697957

Journal Reference:
Chris Carbone, Tom Maddox, Paul J Funston, Michael G.L Mills, Gregory F Grether, Blaire Van Valkenburgh Parallels between playbacks and Pleistocene tar seeps suggest sociality in an extinct sabretooth cat, Smilodon Published 23 February 2009.DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0526

Abstract
Inferences concerning the lives of extinct animals are difficult to obtain from the fossil record. Here we present a novel approach to the study of extinct carnivores, using a comparison between fossil records (n=3324) found in Late Pleistocene tar seeps at Rancho La Brea in North America and counts (n=4491) from playback experiments used to estimate carnivore abundance in Africa. Playbacks and tar seep deposits represent competitive, potentially dangerous encounters where multiple predators are lured by dying herbivores. Consequently, in both records predatory mammals and birds far outnumber herbivores. In playbacks, two large social species, lions, Panthera leo, and spotted hyenas, Crocuta crocuta, actively moved towards the sounds of distressed prey and made up 84 per cent of individuals attending. Small social species (jackals) were next most common and solitary species of all sizes were rare. In the La Brea record, two species dominated, the presumably social dire wolf Canis dirus (51%), and the sabretooth cat Smilodon fatalis (33%). As in the playbacks, a smaller social canid, the coyote Canis latrans, was third most common (8%), and known solitary species were rare (

http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/ ... 1.full.pdf

Journal Reference:
Blaire Van Valkenburgh, Tom Maddox, Paul J. Funston, Michael G. L. Mills, Gregory F. Grether, Chris Carbone Sociality in Rancho La Brea Smilodon: arguments favour ‘evidence’ over ‘coincidence’ Published 14 May 2009.DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0261

Kiffner (2009) questions the comparability of the Pleistocene tar seep records and those derived from predator playback experiments. While we acknowledge that they are not identical, the differences are not sufficient to overturn our inference of sociality in Rancho La Brea (RLB) Smilodon. He suggests that a greater likelihood of heavier animals becoming trapped might have significantly biased our results. However, carnivores far outnumber (by 9 : 1) the much heavier herbivores (bison, horse) at RLB. There were also differences in preservation among the carnivores themselves. The most abundant species, the dire wolf, was ranked fourth largest in body mass, and the second most abundant species, Smilodon, was ranked third in body mass. Size bias clearly does not explain the pattern of fossilization at RLB. Kiffner suggests that social carnivores would be less likely to be trapped at RLB, because their larger brain size and presumed greater intelligence would lead them to avoid this when they witnessed trapped fellow group members. Arguing against this are: (i) the large numbers of preserved dire wolves, a species that is assumed to have been social given its ancestry and (ii) little or no support for larger brain size among large, social carnivores (Perez-Barberia et al. 2007).

Several criticisms raised by Kiffner (2009) concern issues of comparing responses to playback experiments with responses to an actual carcass with associated predators. We agree that the two differ significantly; the tar seep events went on for hours or days until the carcass was gone, whereas the playbacks ran for brief intervals of time. This would be expected to result in a greater species diversity at RLB, as subordinate species could visit the carcass after the larger, dominant species had left. Indeed, this is the case, with weasels, badgers, skunks, bobcats and foxes present at RLB but with very few similar species observed in the playback experiments. Kiffner points out that a carcass would provide olfactory cues as well as auditory and visual cues, whereas playbacks would supply only the latter two, but does not suggest how this might have biased the RLB records. We do not see how this is relevant to the question of Smilodon sociality.

He also notes that the response of species to playbacks is affected by how long a recording is played, and the ‘type of audio lure’, which we assume to mean the content of the recording (hyena calls versus herbivore in distress). Lions may be slower than hyenas to show up at playbacks, in part because lions walk and hyenas lope. Given this, the open-ended RLB events should have done an excellent job of attracting both quicker and slower species, and this supports our hypothesis that some species, such as pumas and jaguars, and possibly grey wolves, avoided attending, just as cheetahs avoid playbacks. Similarly, if ‘type of audio lure’ influences the proportion of species at playbacks, this would not have been an issue at RLB, as both types of calls, those of distressed prey and their squabbling predators, would have been present. Notably, the African playback experiments we used differed from one another in type of audio lure, time of day and duration, but there was no significant difference between them in the distribution of species that responded by size and sociality (Carbone et al. 2009).

Finally, Kiffner (2009) questions our assumption of broad similarity between the African savannah predator guild and that of the late Pleistocene of Los Angeles, California, and suggests that other modern carnivore guilds, such as those that include tigers, might be more appropriate. Unfortunately, there are no playback data for regions that include tigers but we would be interested in comparing such results, especially for regions where the social dhole is abundant. Nevertheless, African savannah or open woodland ecosystems are a more suitable choice for comparison with RLB than any extant forest ecosystem of the Indian subcontinent or elsewhere. In both Africa and RLB, the dominant large herbivores are herd-forming grazers, such as antelope, zebra and buffalo in Africa versus camel, horse and bison in California. These ungulates indicate that RLB was an open woodland, and this is consistent with palaeobotanical and isotopic evidence (Stock & Harris 1992; Coltrain et al. 2004). Although difficult to quantify, it appears likely that encounters over carcasses such as those which occurred at RLB are more likely in relatively open environments with greater visibility and soaring vultures that signal the presence of a carcass. We note that the diversity of large, soaring, scavenging birds is similar at RLB and in African savannah (Stock & Harris 1992). It is difficult to know how tigers would behave in an open environment, where they are part of a more diverse predator guild that includes species larger than themselves as well as a hyena analogue. Clearly, tigers exhibit some social flexibility under extreme scenarios, but, like many carnivores, might be expected to alter their behaviour according to circumstances affecting resource availability and levels of competition. Smilodon may have been solitary in parts of its range, but it seems unlikely that it was so in the highly competitive setting found at RLB in the late Pleistocene.


http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/ ... 3.full.pdf
Last edited by Taipan on January 23rd, 2018, 4:21 am, edited 1 time in total.
Quote
Like
Share